Skip to main content

Effects of movement behaviors on preschoolers’ cognition: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials

Abstract

Background

Movement behaviors, including physical activity (PA), sedentary behavior (SB), and sleep, are fundamental to early childhood development. These behaviors interact dynamically within a 24-hour period, creating a complex balance that influences not only physical health but also cognitive and emotional well-being in young children. While the physical health benefits of movement behaviors are well-documented, systematic evaluations of how interventions targeting these behaviors affect cognitive development in preschool-aged children remain limited.

Methods

This review was guided through PRISMA 2020 guidelines. We conducted a systematic review of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) to evaluate the impact of interventions targeting PA, SB, and sleep on cognitive outcomes in preschool-aged children. A comprehensive search was performed across five databases: PubMed, PsycInfo, Web of Science, Embase, and CINAHL, covering studies published between January 2000 and December 2023. Eligible studies were those that focused on at least one movement behavior, had a minimum intervention duration of four weeks, and assessed cognitive development as a primary outcome. The cognitive outcomes evaluated included executive function, attention, memory, and other key domains critical to early childhood development, such as language, processing speed, and social cognition.

Results

Twenty-two RCTs (14 individual, 8 cluster) met the inclusion criteria. Of these, 21 studies focused on PA, while only one targeted SB, and none specifically addressed sleep or combined movement behaviors. PA interventions, particularly those involving cognitively engaging activities, significantly improved cognitive domains such as executive function, inhibition, and attention, with effect sizes ranging from moderate to large (Cohen’s d > 0.5). The SB-focused study did not report significant cognitive improvements. A clear gap exists in understanding the effects of sleep and multi-behavior interventions on cognitive outcomes.

Conclusions

Cognitively engaging PA interventions demonstrated the largest effects, while motor skill-focused and general PA programs produced moderate to smaller gains. Evidence on SB and sleep interventions remains limited, with no studies exploring the combined effects of these three movement behaviors. Future research should focus on integrated interventions that address PA, SB, and sleep to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of their collective impact on cognitive development in early childhood.

Trial registration

This study was registered with PROSPERO under the registration number CRD42023479156.

Background

Movement behaviors span a spectrum from sleep to vigorous physical activity (PA) [1]. Across a 24-hour period, individuals allocate varying amounts of time to PA, sedentary behavior (SB), and sleep. As time spent in one behavior increases, the time available for others decreases [2]. This creates a dynamic interaction that impacts overall health and development [3, 4]. Early childhood, particularly the preschool years (ages 3 to 5), is a critical period for developing key movement behaviors, including regular PA, adequate sleep, and limiting SB [5]. Establishing these behaviors during this period supports healthy growth and development [6]. In response to the growing recognition of these behaviors’ importance, various countries and global organizations like Canada and the World Health Organization (WHO) have introduced“24-hour Movement Guidelines for the Early Years” [7,8,9,10,11,12]. These guidelines provide recommendations for PA, SB, and sleep, and highlight the importance of movement behaviors in promoting overall health and well-being during early childhood.

The preschool years are an important period for brain plasticity, neurodevelopment, and the establishment of behavioral patterns [13]. During this time, children acquire essential skills that form the foundation for later academic achievement and overall well-being [14]. Cognitive competencies such as attention, memory, language, and executive function develop rapidly during this time and are influenced by numerous factors, including movement behaviors (i.e., PA, SB, and sleep) [15,16,17,18,19]. Movement behaviors established during this period are more likely to continue into later life, promoting lasting cognitive and health benefits. Early childhood, therefore, presents a unique opportunity to establish movement habits that may have enduring effects on cognitive development. This emphasizes the importance of understanding how movement behaviors contribute to cognitive development in early years.

Independently, PA has demonstrated significant benefits for neurocognitive development by enhancing brain plasticity, promoting synaptic growth, and supporting cognitive functions such as attention and executive functioning [20]. In contrast, excessive SB, particularly screen time, has been associated with cognitive deficits, including delayed language development and reduced executive functioning [21]. Adequate sleep also plays a crucial role in brain development, with higher sleep quality and sufficient duration contributing to improved memory consolidation, attention, and emotional regulation [22]. Despite evidence suggesting that PA may benefit cognitive domains in early childhood [23, 24], the role of PA in academic-related outcomes remains inconsistent due to variability in study designs and interventions [25]. Furthermore, evidence specific to SB and sleep in preschool-aged children is sparse, and the impact of interventions targeting these behaviors on cognitive outcomes is less understood. This highlights the need for further systematic reviews to better understand how interventions focused on individual movement behaviors affect cognitive development in young children, including the identification of optimal types, durations, and intensities.

Collectively, movement guidelines emphasize the integration of PA, SB, and sleep, recognizing that these behaviors interact dynamically and compete for time within a finite 24-hour period [26]. Since an increase in one behavior typically requires a decrease in another, examining these behaviors in isolation may yield incomplete conclusions [27, 28]. For instance, if an intervention successfully increases a child’s PA by 30 min, that time must be offset by reducing other movement behaviors. Increasing PA often reduces SB or alters sleep patterns, while inadequate sleep can negatively impact both cognitive functioning and the ability to engage in PA. Conversely, excessive SB may displace opportunities for PA or contribute to poor sleep quality. These behaviors are interdependent; changes in one can influence the others, collectively affecting physical health and cognitive outcomes [29, 30]. Therefore, adopting an integrated and balanced approach is critical to understanding how these movement behaviors interact and collectively impact health and cognition. However, evidence on the effects of combined movement behavior interventions on cognitive development in the early years remains limited.

Given that children’s daily behavioral experiences are crucial for their brain and cognitive development, understanding how PA, SB, and sleep independently and collectively influence cognition is critical [31]. Achieving recommendations for all three movement behaviors is vital for children’s health and development [32]. Numerous studies, to date, have examined the individual effects of movement behaviors on cognitive outcomes in preschoolers, but the findings are varied due to differences in study designs and methodologies. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs), known for their ability to minimize bias and establish causality, provide a reliable method for assessing these effects [33]. Additionally, while movement guidelines emphasize the interconnectedness of PA, SB, and sleep, few reviews have systematically explored these behaviors in an integrated manner, particularly in the context of preschool-aged children. Without this knowledge, it remains challenging to fully understand how these movement behaviors collectively influence cognitive development during this critical period. Therefore, this systematic review aims to critically evaluate the existing evidence from RCTs that target PA, SB, and sleep, both individually and in combination, to understand their effects on cognitive outcomes in early childhood. By synthesizing findings from high-quality studies, this review seeks to provide clearer guidance on the influence of movement behaviors on cognitive outcomes, identify optimal movement patterns that support cognitive development during this critical period, and inform evidence-based interventions tailored to this age group.

Methods

Study protocol and registration

This review was guided through the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) 2020 statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses [34]. The search strategies were developed by two reviewers (NZ and CP), including databases, search terms, search date range, language restrictions, types of articles, and inclusion criteria. The strategies were constructed in consultation with a trained reference librarian and search specialist at the University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center. This review was registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/; Registration no CRD42023479156).

Search strategy

This review aimed to examine the impacts of movement behaviors (i.e., PA, SB, and sleep) on broader cognitive outcomes in preschool-aged children. The databases PubMed, PsycInfo, Web of Science, Embase, and CINAHL were used for the search, which was conducted between December 2023 and January 2024. The search terms were designed in line with the PICO strategy (Population – preschool children; Intervention – movement behaviors; Comparison – control group or normal activity; Outcome – cognition). To ensure comprehensive results, a combination of Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and related keywords was employed. Where possible, database filters were applied to refine the results, although filter availability varied across platforms. PsycInfo, in particular, does not provide an easy filter for RCTs, so we utilized the search strategy recommended by Cochrane to address this limitation [35] (see Appendix A for a full description of the search strategy for each database). All retrieved studies were manually screened by two reviewers (CP and NZ) for relevance. Studies were first screened based on titles and abstracts, and potentially relevant full-text articles were then reviewed. References of relevant articles were also searched for additional results. The identified studies were stored using reference management software (EndNote 20, Thomson Reuters, New York, NY, USA).

Inclusion criteria

Eligibility criteria required that studies (1) were published in English in peer-reviewed journals between January 2000 and December 2023; (2) employed a RCT design (individual or cluster-based); (3) included at least one movement behavior (PA, SB, or sleep), operationalized in alignment with the Canadian 24-hour Movement Guidelines [10]; (4) had a minimum intervention duration of four weeks, as research suggests this length is sufficient to produce positive mental and physical health outcomes [36]; (5) assessed cognition as a primary outcome. Cognition was broadly defined as a mental process by which an individual acquires knowledge or understanding, contributing to intellect, memory, or perception [24]. Broader aspects of cognition were reviewed, included but not limited to: academic achievement and/or performance, executive functions, learning, language, concentration/attention, memory, spatial processing, processing speed, emotion regulation, social cognition, creativity, and intelligence quotient (IQ), etc [37]. ; (6) included normatively developing preschool children, with an average sample age between 3 and 6 years old [24]; (7) utilized interventions that targeted the child directly, rather than focusing on caregivers; and (8) implemented interventions in the child’s home or school setting (e.g., childcare, early childhood education centers, Head Start programs, kindergarten, etc.).

Synthesis

A pilot literature selection was conducted to ensure high inter-rater reliability among the reviewers. Following the inclusion and exclusion criteria, the lead author (CP) initially retrieved all relevant articles. A list of relevant articles was created in a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA) and shared among all authors. All four authors independently completed two rounds of screening. In the first round, titles, abstracts, and keywords were reviewed for relevance after removing duplicates. In the second round, full-text screening was performed for articles deemed potentially eligible or with unclear eligibility. Each study was rigorously evaluated against pre-established criteria, and any conflicts were resolved through discussion until a consensus was reached. The selection process was documented in detail and summarized in a PRISMA flow diagram. Two independent reviewers (CP and NZ) extracted data from each article using a pre-specified form, which included: general information (publication date, first author, study location), participant characteristics (sample size, average age, sex), intervention setting; type; and dosage (duration, intensity, frequency), control conditions (e.g., waitlist, no intervention, or maintained routine/recess), cognitive outcomes (domains assessed), assessment tools, and main cognitive findings related to movement behaviors (see Table 1). Reviewers then compared extracted data and resolved any discrepancies through discussion until consensus was achieved.

Table 1 Study characteristics

Effect size calculations

It is important to note that relying solely on p-values to interpret research findings has limitations, as p-values indicate statistical significance but do not convey the magnitude or practical importance of an effect. Moreover, p-values are sensitive to sample size, which can lead to potentially misleading conclusions, such as overestimating significance in larger samples or underestimating it in smaller ones [60]. To address this, we calculated effect sizes for interventions with statistically significant results, including all significant cognitive outcomes identified in the studies.

Effect sizes provide a standardized measure of the magnitude of the effect, independent of sample size [61]. This approach enables better quantification and comparison of the effects across studies, a clearer understanding of intervention impacts, and facilitates the practical interpretation and application of findings. Specifically, we computed Cohen’s d to interpret effect size. Data on pre-test and post-test means and standard deviations were extracted from the results sections and were calculated using the formula: [d = (Meanpost - Meanpre)/SDpooled], where the pooled standard deviation (SD) was computed as: [SDpooled = sqrt((SDpre^2 + SDpost^2)/2)] [62]. Cohen’s d was interpreted as small (0.2), medium (0.5), or large (0.8) [62].

For studies reporting follow-up assessments, only post-test scores were used for effect size calculations to avoid the influence of time-related factors and ensure a consistent measure of immediate intervention impacts. All calculations were performed using RStudio (version 2024.04.2) (RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA, http://www.rstudio.com/).

Risk of bias

The Revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomized trials (RoB 2) [63] and the Revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for cluster-randomized trials (RoB 2 CRT) [64] were used to assess the quality of individually randomized parallel-group trials and cluster-randomized parallel-group trials, respectively. These tools evaluate each study based on multiple criteria, including the randomization process, adherence to the intended intervention, allocation concealment, blinding (during both the study and outcome assessment phases), completeness of outcome data, selective reporting, and other potential sources of bias. Each study was assigned an overall risk-of-bias rating of “low,” “high,” or “unclear.” All authors independently assessed the quality of the studies, and any disagreements were resolved through discussion until consensus was reached. The interrater reliability was measured using Cohen’s kappa, which resulted in a score of 0.78, indicating substantial agreement among the reviewers.

Results

Search results

The initial search yielded 978 results. After removing duplicates, 756 records were screened based on titles and abstracts. Of these, 566 articles were excluded for not meeting relevance criteria, leaving 190 reports for full-text retrieval. After further excluding 169 articles that did not meet the inclusion criteria, 21 studies remained from the initial search. An additional study was identified through reference list searches, bringing the total number of included studies to 22. Of these, 14 were individual RCTs, and 8 were cluster RCTs (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram of report selection

Study characteristics

The studies included in this review were published between 2015 and 2023, representing a wide geographical range. These studies were conducted in eight countries: Italy (2 studies) [38, 46], USA (4 studies) [39, 43, 48, 54], Tunisia (2 studies) [40, 41], China (4 studies) [42, 44, 45, 47], Australia (7 studies) [49,50,51,52,53, 55, 56], Spain (1 study) [57], Switzerland (1 study) [58], and Portugal (1 study) [59]. Participants’ ages ranged from approximately 3.8 to 5.84 years, with sample sizes varying between 45 and 486 children. The proportion of boys across studies ranged from 40 to 60%, reflecting balanced gender representation. The diversity in participant characteristics and geographical distribution enhances the generalizability of the findings across different cultural and demographic contexts.

Intervention characteristics

All included studies were conducted in structured school-based environments such as early childcare centers, kindergartens, or preschools, ensuring consistency in intervention implementation and participant engagement. Across the 22 studies, a wide variety of intervention types, durations, frequencies, and session lengths were documented. PA was the predominant focus (21 studies), with only one study targeting SB [49] and none exclusively addressing sleep or multi-behavior interventions. PA interventions ranged from gross motor activities (e.g., jumping, running, and dance) to cognitively engaging tasks designed to improve executive function (e.g., “Simon Says”-type games or yoga).

The average intervention duration was approximately 10.1 weeks, with the shortest lasting 4 weeks and the longest spanning a full academic year. Session frequencies varied from 1 to 5 times per week, with most studies implementing 2 to 3 sessions per week. The duration of each session ranged from 10 to 60 min, with an average session length of approximately 30 min (average = 28.68 min).

Control or comparison groups primarily maintained regular activity (11 studies), participated in free play (2 studies), or attended standard PE classes (2 studies). Other controls included no PA (1 study), outdoor recess (1 study), and traditional sedentary classroom teaching (2 studies). Certain studies utilized multiple control groups, such as a combination of free play and maintained regular activities.

Outcomes of interest and their assessments

The cognitive outcomes assessed across the studies varied widely, with inconsistent measures utilized to evaluate similar constructs. Executive function was the most frequently assessed domain and is widely understood to encompass core components such as inhibition, working memory, and shifting. For example, the Early Years Toolbox “Go/No-Go” task was frequently used to measure inhibition, while the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) test assessed cognitive flexibility and shifting. Some tools, such as the Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders (HTKS) test, are used to assess multiple executive function components and behavioral aspects of self-regulation simultaneously. In this review, we adopted a broad framework for executive function to capture the diversity of approaches used in the included studies, while acknowledging that heterogeneity in its operationalization could complicate comparisons.

Other cognitive outcomes assessed included numeracy skills (e.g., counting) and language and literacy (e.g., linguistic comprehension, pre-literacy skills, and cognitive abilities related to reading and writing). Memory outcomes included free recall and cued recall, while attention outcomes, such as visual attention, attention span, and reaction time, were assessed separately from executive function. Social-emotional development outcomes, such as hyperactivity and inattention, were also measured independently and were not categorized under executive function in most studies.

These variations in outcome classification and measurement highlight the methodological heterogeneity across the studies, reflecting differences in conceptual frameworks and tools used to assess cognitive domains. This methodological diversity, particularly in the assessment of executive function, emphasizes the need for standardized frameworks in future research. Although it is theoretically possible to conduct a meta-analysis on the cognitive outcomes reported, the substantial heterogeneity in assessment methods introduces significant biases and limits the validity and reliability of any meta-analytic conclusions. Consequently, neither a meta-analysis nor a subgroup analysis was feasible for this review.

Intervention effects

Of the 22 included studies, 19 reported statistically significant improvements in cognitive outcomes following PA interventions, while three found no significant effects. Table 2 displays the effect sizes for these 19 effective interventions, showing that PA interventions integrating cognitive engagement, such as executive function-focused games and integrated physical activities with learning components, produced the largest effect sizes (Cohen’s d > 0.8). For example, Giordano & Alesi (2022) observed a large effect size in inhibition tasks (Cohen’s d = 2.33 for the Day-Night Stroop Task) following a cognitively engaging PA intervention. This program involved replacing previously learned movements, such as substituting walking on tiptoes with walking on heels, in 20-minute sessions three times per week over six weeks [38]. Bai et al. (2022) utilized executive function-focused exercise games, such as “Do the Opposite” and “Flip the Cards,” delivered in 50-minute sessions three times per week for eight weeks. This intervention yielded large effect sizes for inhibitory control, working memory, and shifting tasks [47]. This intervention yielded large effect sizes in inhibitory control, working memory, and shifting tasks. Additionally, Mavilidi et al. (2018) employed integrated physical activities where children engaged in running, jumping, and stepping on numbers in a game-like setting. Conducted in 15-minute sessions once per week for four weeks, the intervention demonstrated significant improvements in numeracy skills and cognitive performance (Cohen’s d > 0.8) [52]. These findings highlight the advantages of integrating cognitive and physical components into intervention programs to achieve meaningful cognitive gains.

Table 2 Effect size calculations for significant cognitive outcomes across all interventions

Moderate effect sizes (Cohen’s d = 0.5–0.8) were observed in interventions emphasizing gross motor activities, progressive muscle relaxation, and exergame-based PA. For instance, Liu et al. (2022) reported moderate improvements in working memory (Cohen’s d = 0.7) and attention-shifting tasks (Cohen’s d = 0.5) through exergame dance movements delivered four times per week for four weeks [42]. Similarly, Hudson et al. (2021) achieved moderate effects in numeracy skills (Cohen’s d = 0.54) and inhibitory control (Cohen’s d = 0.53) following an intervention combining gross and fine motor skill activities conducted twice weekly over eight weeks [39]. Jarraya et al. (2022) also demonstrated moderate improvements in visuomotor precision (Cohen’s d = 0.54) using progressive muscle relaxation sessions delivered twice per week for 12 weeks [41]. These studies illustrate the effectiveness of moderate-intensity PA interventions that combine PA with cognitive elements in improving specific cognitive outcomes.

Interventions with small effect sizes (Cohen’s d < 0.5) generally focused on narrow domains such as linguistic comprehension, specific numeracy tasks, and inhibitory control. For example, Olive et al. (2023) implemented the Active Early Learning program, consisting of 3–5 sessions per week over 22 weeks, which produced small improvements in inhibition tasks (Cohen’s d = 0.54) [55]. Likewise, Shen et al. (2020) employed street-dance training involving 40–50-minute sessions three times per week over eight weeks, achieving small effect sizes in cognitive flexibility tasks (Cohen’s d = 0.4) [44]. While these interventions demonstrated limited impact, they offered incremental cognitive benefits, particularly in specialized areas such as working memory and cognitive shifting. The modest effects observed may stem from limited intervention intensity or frequency, such as 6-week programs with two 20-minute sessions per week, which may not provide sufficient exposure for robust cognitive changes. Additionally, interventions targeting single domains, such as motor inhibition or cognitive flexibility, may lack the holistic stimulation required for broader cognitive development. The choice of activities, like street-dance training, might not fully align with children’s interests or cognitive needs, reducing intrinsic motivation and engagement. These findings underscore the importance of tailoring interventions to children’s developmental and cognitive profiles, increasing their intensity and duration, and ensuring that activities align with targeted cognitive outcomes for greater effectiveness.

Effect sizes, overall, calculated for significant cognitive outcomes, highlighted the pronounced impact of cognitively engaging PA interventions on executive function domains, including inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility in preschool-aged children. However, the lack of evidence on SB and sleep interventions highlights critical gaps in understanding the broader role of 24-hour movement behaviors in cognitive development.

RoB Assessment

The overall quality of the 14 individual RCTs was low, with five studies rated as having “some concerns” and three rated as having “high” RoB. None were rated as having “low” RoB. Similarly, the quality of the eight cluster RCTs was also low, with all studies rated as having “some concerns.” It is important to note that the Cochrane RoB tool is highly rigorous, making it particularly challenging for non-clinical behavioral interventions (where subjective influence is difficult to control) to achieve a “low” risk rating. For detailed assessments of each study and the overall RoB across different domains, see Figs. 2 and 3, and 4.

Fig. 2
figure 2

ROB2-individual

Fig. 3
figure 3

ROB2-Cluster

Fig. 4
figure 4

ROB2-overall

Discussion

The 24-Hour Movement Guidelines emphasize the importance of balancing PA, SB (including screen time), and sleep throughout the day. The combination of these movement behaviors plays a critical role in shaping overall health in young children [10]. The preschool years are a critical period for cognitive development, and there is growing interest in identifying effective interventions to enhance cognitive skills during this time. This review synthesizes evidence from recent RCTs that examine the effects of interventions targeting movement behaviors, either individually or in combination, on various aspects of cognitive development in early childhood. We included 14 individual RCTs and 8 cluster RCTs and observed that movement behaviors, particularly PA interventions with a cognitively engaging element, have a positive impact on various aspects of cognition in preschoolers. However, the effects of interventions targeting SB, sleep, or combinations of these behaviors on cognition were less conclusive due to the limited number of studies in these areas.

Our findings are consistent with previous reviews, which suggest that PA interventions have a positive effect on cognitive development in early childhood, while also highlighting subtle differences across study designs and outcomes. For instance, both Carson et al. (2016) [23] and Zeng et al. (2017) [24] demonstrate that PA interventions consistently yield statistically significant benefits for cognitive outcomes, particularly in executive function, attention, and language, which aligns with the results of our review. However, Carson’s review included both observational and experimental studies, offering a broader perspective on PA’s influence on cognition. In contrast, our review focuses exclusively on RCTs, providing stronger causal evidence of PA’s beneficial effects on cognitive development in young children. Additionally, while Zeng’s review included fewer RCTs and did not explore the dose-response relationship between PA and cognition, our review addresses this gap by providing a more detailed analysis of intervention characteristics, such as duration, frequency, and intensity. Moreover, Li et al. (2020) [36] conducted a meta-analysis demonstrating that chronic PA interventions positively influenced executive functions, including inhibition and working memory, in young children, which aligns with the findings of our review. Notably, Li’s study highlighted that the type of PA (e.g., PA-only vs. PA combined with cognitive challenges) moderated the effects on executive functions. Our review expands on this by emphasizing the enhanced benefits of cognitively engaging PA activities over and above simply being physically active, which were shown to improve outcomes in certain trials. This suggests that integrating cognitive tasks within PA may yield stronger effects on cognitive development compared to PA alone. Similarly, the most recent meta-analysis by Morales et al. (2024) [65] found that PA interventions positively impacted various cognitive outcomes, including attention, inhibition, and working memory, further supporting our findings [66]. Interestingly, Morales’s review also noted that interventions lasting longer than three weeks were most effective, reinforcing our assumption that longer intervention durations might lead to more substantial cognitive benefits, as our review included only interventions lasting more than four weeks.

Our review identified a significant underrepresentation of interventions targeting SB, which limits the conclusiveness of findings in this area. The single study that focused on SB reported no significant cognitive improvements, suggesting that reducing sedentary time alone may not be sufficient to enhance cognitive outcomes in young children [49]. One explanation for the lack of significant cognitive improvements may be the passive nature of SB reduction strategies. Standing tables and increased activity during mealtimes may not be sufficiently stimulating for cognitive development compared to more interactive and physically engaging interventions. In contrast, interventions incorporating both physical and cognitive challenges, such as those involving active play or structured exercise programs, have been shown to produce more substantial cognitive gains in children than passive strategies [67,68,69]. This suggests that simply reducing sedentary time without incorporating stimulating activities may fall short of promoting cognitive improvements. This finding aligns with prior research indicating that cognitive benefits in young children may require more comprehensive and engaging approaches rather than simply reducing SB. For instance, a meta-analysis by Carson et al. (2015) found that while increased PA was consistently linked to better cognitive performance in children, interventions focusing solely on reducing SB were less effective in producing cognitive benefits [21]. Therefore, future interventions to limit SB may benefit from incorporating more engaging activities, possibly integrating cognitive challenges to enhance cognitive benefits.

Emerging evidence highlights the critical role of sleep in cognitive development, particularly in processes such as memory consolidation, synaptic pruning, and emotional regulation [70]. Sleep facilitates the transfer of information from short-term to long-term memory, a process essential for learning and knowledge retention [66, 71]. Adequate sleep duration and quality are further associated with improved attention, executive functioning, and emotional stability [72]. In early childhood, these cognitive functions are foundational, underpinning the acquisition of language, self-regulation, problem-solving skills, and social interactions [73, 74]. Despite such evidence, sleep interventions for preschool-aged children remain underexplored, with no RCTs specifically targeting sleep outcomes in the context of cognitive development identified in our review. This absence represents a significant gap, as studies involving older children have shown that sleep hygiene interventions can enhance both academic performance and behavioral outcomes [75]. Therefore, investigating the effects of structured sleep interventions such as establishing consistent bedtime routines, reducing screen time before sleep, and maintaining a regular sleep schedule, on the cognitive development of preschoolers could provide invaluable insights.

We failed to locate any RCTs that examined the effects of combined movement behaviors on cognitive outcomes in young children. Current literature suggests that interventions addressing multiple aspects of 24-hour movement behaviors may offer comprehensive and synergistic benefits for cognitive health. For instance, a meta-analysis by Song et al. (2022) demonstrated that integrating interventions focused on both sleep and PA can yield greater cognitive benefits in children compared to targeting a single behavior alone [76]. This aligns with a growing body of evidence suggesting that these behaviors are not independent but interact in complex ways that affect brain development and function. A recent review by Wang et al. (2024) also highlighted the potential for combined interventions across 24-hour movement behaviors to positively influence mental health and cognitive performance in children and adolescents [77]. The review underscored the importance of considering the interplay between these behaviors, advocating for more RCTs that simultaneously address PA, SB, and sleep. Such studies could more accurately reflect the holistic nature of how movement behaviors contribute to cognitive development in young children, particularly in a real-world context where these behaviors are interrelated. Although research in this area is still emerging, early evidence supports the benefits of a multi-behavior approach. For example, a recent parent-focused RCT targeting all three 24-hour movement behaviors in preschool children found that incorporating sleep, PA, and SB management led to improvements in behavioral outcomes [78]. While the study focused on behavioral health, it did not include direct measures of cognitive outcomes. This leaves a gap in understanding how combined 24-hour movement behaviors might affect cognitive function in early childhood. The lack of RCTs investigating the cognitive outcomes of multi-behavior interventions points to an urgent need for future research. By designing studies that address all aspects of 24-hour movement behaviors, researchers could discover new ways to support cognitive development during early childhood, a critical time when the brain is highly adaptable. With the known benefits of sleep and PA for brain health, and growing evidence of the harms of too much SB, there is a strong reason to explore these connections further.

Strengths and limitations

A key strength of this review is its exclusive focus on RCTs, which are considered the gold standard for establishing causal relationships between interventions and outcomes. Additionally, focusing exclusively on studies with cognition as a primary outcome strengthens the validity of our findings by eliminating ambiguity and ensuring that cognitive improvements were the central objective of the included interventions. Furthermore, we comprehensively reviewed all movement behaviors and their combinations, despite most studies focusing primarily on PA interventions. To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review investigating the causal relationship between movement behaviors and cognitive outcomes in young children. We also calculated effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for interventions with statistically significant results, providing a standardized measure of intervention impact independent of sample size. This approach facilitates better comparisons across studies and offers clearer insights into the efficacy of interventions on cognitive outcomes. However, several limitations should be noted. First, the conceptualization of cognition in this review may not have captured all relevant outcomes. Cognition is a broad and loosely defined construct, making it challenging to establish a universally accepted definition or include every potential keyword in the search. Second, the heterogeneity in intervention types, durations, cognitive measures, and assessment tools across studies complicates efforts to synthesize the findings through meta-analysis or sub-analyses, which limits our ability to quantify overall effect sizes. To address these challenges, future research should prioritize standardized assessment tools and harmonized outcome measures. Consistent measurement approaches will enhance the comparability of findings across studies, enabling more meaningful subgroup analyses and more robust evidence synthesis. Another notable limitation of this review is the RoB, with many studies rated as having “some concerns” or “high risk.” Factors such as lack of blinding and issues with allocation concealment contribute to the overall low quality of the included studies. This limitation emphasizes the need for cautious interpretation and call for future trials to implement standardized, blinded methodologies to minimize bias and enhance reliability. As studies reporting significant or positive findings are more likely to be published and included in systematic reviews, publication bias could be a potential limitation as well. While we conducted a comprehensive search to include studies with null or non-significant results, the possibility of bias in the available evidence cannot be entirely ruled out. Last but not least, our search may not be fully comprehensive, as it was limited to several key databases. Relevant databases such as SPORTDiscus were not included, which may have resulted in the exclusion of some potential articles on the topic. This limitation highlights the possibility of missing studies that could have contributed to the findings of this review.

Conclusions

This review demonstrates that PA interventions significantly enhance cognitive outcomes in early childhood, particularly in executive functions such as inhibition, attention, and cognitive flexibility. Interventions incorporating cognitively engaging activities showed the largest effects, while motor skill-focused and general PA programs yielded moderate to smaller gains. Specific cognitive skills, such as linguistic comprehension and numeracy, showed incremental improvements, highlighting the variability of outcomes based on intervention type and design. These findings highlight the importance of early implementation of tailored PA interventions to support cognitive development in young children. Optimizing outcomes requires careful consideration of the type, intensity, and frequency of interventions. However, further research is needed to investigate the effects of SB, sleep, and combinations of movement behaviors, offering a more comprehensive understanding of their collective role in early cognitive development.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Abbreviations

CINAHL:

Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature

DCCS:

Dimensional Change Card Sort

HTKS:

Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders

IQ:

Intelligence Quotient

MeSH:

Medical Subject Headings

PA:

Physical Activity

PICO:

Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcome

PRISMA:

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses

RCT:

Randomized Controlled Trial

RoB:

Risk of Bias

SB:

Sedentary Behavior

SD:

Standard Deviation

SEM:

Standard Error of the Mean

WHO:

World Health Organization

References

  1. Carson V, Faulkner G, Sabiston CM, Tremblay MS, Leatherdale ST. Patterns of movement behaviors and their association with overweight and obesity in youth. Int J Public Health. 2015;60:551–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Rosenberger ME, Fulton JE, Buman MP, Troiano RP, Grandner MA, Buchner DM, et al. The 24-Hour activity cycle: a new paradigm for physical activity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019;51:454.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Christian H, Murray K, Trost SG, Schipperijn J, Trapp G, Maitland C, et al. Meeting the Australian 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years is associated with better social-emotional development in preschool boys. Prev Med Rep. 2022;27: 101770.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Vale S, Mota J. Adherence to 24-hour movement guidelines among Portuguese preschool children: the prestyle study. J Sports Sci. 2020;38:2149–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Willumsen J, Bull F. Development of WHO guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep for children less than 5 years of age. J Phys Activity Health. 2020;17:96–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Jones RA, Hinkley T, Okely AD, Salmon J. Tracking physical activity and sedentary behavior in childhood: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2013;44:651–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Sit less, move more, sleep well: active play guidelines for under-fives. Ministry of Health NZ. Available from: https://www.health.govt.nz/publication/sit-less-move-more-sleep-well-active-play-guidelines-under-fives. Cited 2024 Apr 8.

  8. Okely AD, Ghersi D, Hesketh KD, Santos R, Loughran SP, Cliff DP, et al. A collaborative approach to adopting/adapting guidelines - the Australian 24-Hour Movement guidelines for the early years (birth to 5 years): an integration of physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep. BMC Public Health. 2017;17:869.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Loo BKG, Okely A, Taylor R, Novotny R, Wickramasinghe P, Müller-Riemenschneider F, et al. Asia-pacific consensus statement on integrated 24-hour activity guidelines for the early years. Lancet Reg Health West Pac. 2023;32:100641.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Tremblay MS, Chaput J-P, Adamo KB, Aubert S, Barnes JD, Choquette L, et al. Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years (0–4 years): an integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. BMC Public Health. 2017;17:874.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. World Health Organization. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. 2019. Available from: https://www.who.int/publications-detail-redirect/9789241550536. Cited 2020 Dec 21.

  12. Reilly JJ, Hughes AR, Janssen X, Hesketh KR, Livingstone S, Hill C, et al. GRADE-ADOLOPMENT process to develop 24-Hour Movement Behavior recommendations and Physical Activity guidelines for the Under 5s in the United Kingdom, 2019. J Phys Act Health. 2020;17:101–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Brown TT, Jernigan TL. Brain development during the preschool years. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012;22:313–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Campbell FA, Pungello EP, Miller-Johnson S, Burchinal M, Ramey CT. The development of cognitive and academic abilities: growth curves from an early childhood educational experiment. Dev Psychol. 2001;37:231–42.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S, Argitis G, Boseovski J, et al. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2003;68:i–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Tandon PS, Zhou C, Sallis JF, Cain KL, Frank LD, Saelens BE. Home environment relationships with children’s physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by socioeconomic status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Walker MP. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156:168–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Janssen I, LeBlanc AG. Systematic review of the health benefits of physical activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. 2010;7:40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Carson V, Kuzik N, Hunter S, Wiebe SA, Spence JC, Friedman A, et al. Systematic review of sedentary behavior and cognitive development in early childhood. Prev Med. 2015;78:115–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer LJ, Sadeh A, American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night wakings in infants and young children. Sleep. 2006;29:1263–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Carson V, Hunter S, Kuzik N, Wiebe SA, Spence JC, Friedman A, et al. Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19:573–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Zeng N, Ayyub M, Sun H, Wen X, Xiang P, Gao Z. Effects of physical activity on motor skills and cognitive development in early childhood: a systematic review. Biomed Res Int. 2017;2017:e2760716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. St Laurent CW, Burkart S, Andre C, Spencer RMC. Physical activity, fitness, school readiness, and cognition in early childhood: a systematic review. J Phys Act Health. 2021;18:1004–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Rollo S, Antsygina O, Tremblay MS. The whole day matters: understanding 24-hour movement guideline adherence and relationships with health indicators across the lifespan. J Sport Health Sci. 2020;9:493–510.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Chastin SFM, Palarea-Albaladejo J, Dontje ML, Skelton DA. Combined effects of time spent in physical activity, sedentary behaviors and sleep on obesity and cardio-metabolic health markers: a novel compositional data analysis approach. PLoS ONE. 2015;10: e0139984.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Pedišić Ž, Dumuid D, Olds T. Integrating sleep, sedentary behaviour, and physical activity research in the emerging field of time-use epidemiology: definitions, concepts, statistical methods, theoretical framework, and future directions. Kinesiology. 2017; Available from: https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/INTEGRATING-SLEEP%2C-SEDENTARY-BEHAVIOUR%2C-AND-IN-THE-Pedi%C5%A1i%C4%87-Dumuid/d203da6bf2335d6f320e29d04b8033d586f352fa. Cited 2024 Sep 23.

  29. Chaput J-P, Gray CE, Poitras VJ, Carson V, Gruber R, Birken CS, et al. Systematic review of the relationships between sleep duration and health indicators in the early years (0–4 years). BMC Public Health. 2017;17:855.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Poitras VJ, Gray CE, Janssen X, Aubert S, Carson V, Faulkner G, et al. Systematic review of the relationships between sedentary behaviour and health indicators in the early years (0–4 years). BMC Public Health. 2017;17:868.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Walsh JJ, Barnes JD, Cameron JD, Goldfield GS, Chaput J-P, Gunnell KE, et al. Associations between 24-hour movement behaviours and global cognition in a cross-sectional sample of US children. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2018;2:783–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Tremblay MS, Carson V, Chaput J-P, Connor Gorber S, Dinh T, Duggan M, et al. Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for children and youth: an integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2016;41:S311-327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d5928.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Eady AM, Wilczynski NL, Haynes RB. PsycINFO search strategies identified methodologically sound therapy studies and review articles for use by clinicians and researchers. J Clin Epidemiol. 2008;61:34–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Li L, Zhang J, Cao M, Hu W, Zhou T, Huang T, et al. The effects of chronic physical activity interventions on executive functions in children aged 3–7 years: a meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. 2020;23:949–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Harvey PD. Domains of cognition and their assessment. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2019;21:227–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Giordano G, Alesi M. Does physical activity improve inhibition in kindergarteners? A pilot study. Percept Mot Skills. 2022;129:1001–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Hudson KN, Ballou HM, Willoughby MT. Short report: improving motor competence skills in early childhood has corollary benefits for executive function and numeracy skills. Dev Sci. 2021;24: e13071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Jarraya S, Wagner M, Jarraya M, Engel FA. 12 weeks of kindergarten-based yoga practice increases visual attention, Visual-Motor Precision and decreases behavior of inattention and hyperactivity in 5-Year-old children. Front Psychol. 2019;10: 796.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Jarraya S, Jarraya M, Engel FA. Kindergarten-based progressive muscle relaxation training enhances attention and executive functioning: a Randomized Controlled Trial. Percept Mot Skills. 2022;129:644–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Liu Z-M, Chen C-Q, Fan X-L, Lin C-C, Ye X-D. Usability and effects of a combined physical and cognitive intervention based on active Video games for Preschool Children. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19: 7420.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Mulvey KL, Taunton S, Pennell A, Brian A, Head. Toes, knees, SKIP! Improving Preschool Children’s executive function through a motor competence intervention. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2018;40:233–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Shen Y, Zhao Q, Huang Y, Liu G, Fang L. Promotion of street-dance training on the executive function in preschool children. Front Psychol. 2020;11. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.585598. Cited 2022 Aug 22.

  45. Wen X, Zhang Y, Gao Z, Jie J, Bao L. Effect of mini-trampoline physical activity on executive functions in preschool children. BioMed Res Int. 2018;2018:2712803.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Alesi M, Costa S, Bianco A, Pepi A. A teacher-led motor programme to enhance pre-literacy and motor skills in kindergarten children. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2021;18:367–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Bai J, Huang H, Ouyang H. Effects of group-play moderate to vigorous intensity physical activity intervention on executive function and motor skills in 4- to 5-Year-old preschoolers: a pilot cluster randomized controlled trial. Front Psychol. 2022;13. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.847785. Cited 2022 Aug 22.

  48. Burkart S, Roberts J, Davidson MC, Alhassan S. Behavioral effects of a locomotor-based physical activity intervention in preschoolers. J Phys Act Health. 2018;15:46–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Ellis YG, Cliff DP, Howard SJ, Okely AD. Feasibility, acceptability, and potential efficacy of a childcare-based intervention to reduce sitting time among pre-schoolers: a pilot randomised controlled trial. J Sports Sci. 2019;37:146–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Mavilidi M-F, Okely AD, Chandler P, Cliff DP, Paas F. Effects of integrated physical exercises and gestures on preschool children’s foreign language vocabulary learning. Educ Psychol Rev. 2015;27:413–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Mavilidi M-F, Okely AD, Chandler P, Paas F. Effects of integrating physical activities into a science lesson on preschool children’s learning and enjoyment. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2017;31:281–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Mavilidi MF, Okely A, Chandler P, Domazet SL, Paas F. Immediate and delayed effects of integrating physical activity into preschool childrenâ€TMs learning of numeracy skills | elsevier enhanced reader. J Exp Child Psychol. 2018;166:502–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Mavilidi MF, Pesce C, Mazzoli E, Bennett S, Paas F, Okely AD, et al. Effects of cognitively engaging physical activity on preschool children’s cognitive outcomes. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2023;94:839–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Miller AL, Palmer KK, Wang L, Wang C, Riley HO, McClelland MM, et al. Mastery-oriented motor competence intervention improves behavioral but not cognitive self-regulation in head start preschoolers: Randomized controlled trial results. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2023;33:725–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Olive LS, Telford RM, Westrupp E, Telford RD. Physical activity intervention improves executive function and language development during early childhood: the active early learning cluster randomized controlled trial. Child Dev. 2024;95:544–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Piek JP, Kane R, Rigoli D, McLaren S, Roberts CM, Rooney R, et al. Does the Animal Fun program improve social-emotional and behavioural outcomes in children aged 4–6 years? Hum Mov Sci. 2015;43:155–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Sánchez-López M, Cavero‐Redondo I, Álvarez‐Bueno C, Ruiz‐Hermosa A, Pozuelo‐Carrascosa DP, Díez‐Fernández A, et al. Impact of a multicomponent physical activity intervention on cognitive performance: the MOVI‐KIDS study. SCAND J MED SCI SPORTS. 2019;29:766–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Schmidt M, Mavilidi MF, Singh A, Englert C. Combining physical and cognitive training to improve kindergarten children’s executive functions: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2020;63:101908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Veiga G, Guerreiro D, Marmeleira J, Santos GD, Pomar C. OUT to IN: a body-oriented intervention program to promote preschoolers’ self-regulation and relationship skills in the outdoors. Front Psychol. 2023;14: 1195305.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Wasserstein RL, Lazar NA. The ASA Statement on p-Values: context, process, and purpose. Am Stat. 2016;70:129–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Sullivan GM, Feinn R. Using effect size—or why the P value is not enough. J Grad Med Educ. 2012;4:279–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. New York: Routledge; 1988. https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/mono/10.4324/9780203771587/statistical-power-analysis-behavioral-sciencesjacob-cohen.

  63. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;366:l4898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Eldridge SM, Campbell MK, Campbell MJ, Drahota AK, Giraudeau B, Reeves BC, et al. Revised Cochrane risk of bias tool for randomized trials (RoB 2): additional considerations for cluster-randomized trials (RoB 2 CRT). 2021. Available from: https://drive.google.com/file/d/1yDQtDkrp68_8kJiIUdbongK99sx7RFI-/view?usp=sharing&usp=embed_facebook. Cited 2023 Jul 21.

  65. Morales JS, Alberquilla del Río E, Valenzuela PL, Martínez-de-Quel Ó. Physical activity and cognitive performance in early childhood: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of Randomized controlled trials. Sports Med. 2024;54:1835–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Rasch B, Born J. About Sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93:681–766.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev Rev. 2010;30:331–551.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to Aid executive function development in children 4–12 years old. Science. 2011;333:959–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Tomporowski PD, Lambourne K, Okumura MS. Physical activity interventions and children’s mental function: an introduction and overview. Prev Med. 2011;52:S3-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Wei Y, Krishnan GP, Bazhenov M. Synaptic mechanisms of memory consolidation during sleep slow oscillations. J Neurosci. 2016;36:4231–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:114–26.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  72. Bernier A, Beauchamp MH, Bouvette-Turcot A-A, Carlson SM, Carrier J. Sleep and cognition in preschool years: specific links to executive functioning. Child Dev. 2013;84:1542–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Breitenstein RS, Hoyniak CP, McQuillan ME, Bates JE. Sleep and self-regulation in early childhood. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2021;60:111–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Vaughn BE, Elmore-Staton L, Shin N, El-Sheikh M. Sleep as a support for social competence, peer relations, and cognitive functioning in Preschool Children. Behav Sleep Med. 2015;13:92–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Blunden S, Galland B. The complexities of defining optimal sleep: empirical and theoretical considerations with a special emphasis on children. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18:371–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Song W, Feng L, Wang J, Ma F, Chen J, Qu S, et al. Play Smart, be smart? Effect of cognitively engaging physical activity interventions on executive function among children 4 ~ 12 years Old: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Brain Sci. 2022;12:762.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Wang J, Yang Y, Li L, Yang X, Guo X, Yuan X, et al. Comparative efficacy of physical activity types on executive functions in children and adolescents: a network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Sci Med Sport. 2024;27:187–96.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Feng J, Huang WY, Sit CH-P, Reilly JJ, Khan A. Effectiveness of a parent-focused intervention targeting 24-hour movement behaviours in preschool-aged children: a randomised controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. 2024;21:98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was funded by the College of Arts & Sciences and Department of Pediatrics at the University of New Mexico (grant #J0685611). The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, N.Z.; Methodology, C.P., and; N.Z.; Data sorting, C.P., V.C., A.P., and; N.Z.; Writing and original draft preparation, C.P., and; N.Z.; Review and editing, C.P., V.C., A.P., & N.Z. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nan Zeng.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pacheco, C., Culkin, V., Putkaradze, A. et al. Effects of movement behaviors on preschoolers’ cognition: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 22, 12 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12966-025-01705-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12966-025-01705-y

Keywords