- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Contexts, affective and physical states and their variations during physical activity in older adults: an intensive longitudinal study with sensor-triggered event-based ecological momentary assessments
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity volume 22, Article number: 30 (2025)
Abstract
Background
To design effective tailored interventions to promote physical activity (PA) among older adults, insights are needed into the contexts in which older adults engage in PA and their affective and physical experiences. Sensor-triggered event-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) is an innovative method for capturing real-life contexts, as well as affective and physical states, during or immediately after specific events, such as PA. This study aimed to (1) describe the physical and social contexts, and the affective and physical states during PA among older adults, (2) evaluate how these constructs fluctuate during PA episodes, and (3) describe affective states during PA according to the context.
Methods
An intensive longitudinal sensor-triggered event-based EMA study was conducted with 92 Belgian older adults (65 + years). During seven days, participants were monitored using a Fitbit, which triggered a smartphone-based questionnaire on the event-based EMA platform ‘HealthReact’ after a five-minute walk. Participants reported on contexts and affective (positive/negative valence) and physical states (pain and fatigue) during the PA event. Descriptive statistics and generalized mixed models were used for data analysis.
Results
Older adults predominantly engaged in daily physical activities, such as walking for transport, leisure walking, and gardening, rather than structured exercise. They consistently reported high positive affect, low negative affect, and minimal physical complaints during PA. Furthermore, older adults mainly engage in physical activities alone, particularly in outdoor settings. Variations in contexts, affect, and fatigue were mostly driven by within-subject differences. The model showed significant differences across times of day, with negative affect being highest in the evening and fatigue lowest in the morning. Additionally, the physical and social context influenced negative affect (but not positive affect), with outdoor activities performed alone and indoor activities performed with others being associated with lower negative affect.
Conclusions
While these findings could enhance the effectiveness of tailored PA interventions, it remains unclear whether the observed affective and physical states are causes or effects of PA, and whether the contexts in which the activities were performed align with older adults’ preferences. Further research is needed to explore these relationships and to better understand older adults’ preferred PA contexts.
Introduction
Performing regular physical activity (PA) is crucial for maintaining good health and preventing and managing chronic diseases such as cardiovascular disease, type 2 diabetes, stroke, and some types of cancer [1,2,3]. The World Health Organization (WHO) recommends that adults (18–65 years) and older adults (65+) should engage in at least 150 min of moderate-intensity aerobic PA or 75 min of vigorous-intensity aerobic PA per week, or a combination of both [4]. However, adhering to these guidelines tends to decrease with age [5], emphasizing the need to encourage an active lifestyle among older adults. In order to design effective interventions to promote PA among older adults, insights are needed into the physical and social contexts in which older adults engage in PA and their affective and physical experiences during PA.
Despite increasing research demonstrating the influence of both the physical [6, 7], and social environment [8] on PA, little is currently known about the specific contexts in which older adults engage in PA and how these contexts fluctuate and influence their affective and physical states. Furthermore, models based on hedonic motivation (emphasizing pursuit of pleasure and avoidance of pain) [9] and operant conditioning (associating behavior with rewards and punishments) [10] suggest that the affective experiences during and immediately after PA significantly influence future engagement in PA. Affect refers to the broad range of feelings people experience, encompassing both positive and negative states. Russell’s circumplex model of affect provides a theoretical framework for understanding affect along two primary dimensions: valence (ranging from positive to negative) and arousal (ranging from low to high energy) [11]. In this study, we focus on the valence dimension, which captures the degree to which an individual feels pleasant (e.g., happiness) or unpleasant (e.g., anxiety) in response to various contexts. Behaviors that generate desirable affective states, characterized by high levels of positive valence and low levels of negative valence, are more likely to be repeated in the future. In contrast, behaviors that are accompanied with negative affective valence states may discourage future behavior [12,13,14]. Additionally, laboratory research has already demonstrated the link between PA and affect [15, 16], but to date, little is known about the influence of context on this link. A systematic review of ecological momentary assessment studies (EMA) highlights that incidental PA (e.g., gardening, household tasks) at even low intensities often enhances affective well-being, particularly by boosting energy levels, while effects of PA intensity or duration appear inconsistent [17]. These discrepancies likely stem from contextual factors, such as environmental settings, weather conditions and social interactions, which significantly moderate the relationship between PA and affective well-being [17, 18]. In addition, negative physical complaints (e.g., pain and fatigue) are known to reduce older adults’ motivation for PA engagement [19]. However, current understanding of older adults’ affective and physical states during PA, how these fluctuate throughout the day and how they relate to the physical and social contexts in which older adults are physically active is limited.
Existing literature primarily relies on retrospective cross-sectional questionnaires [20], which have shown that activities typically occur in outdoor settings close to older adults’ residences, including venues like parks, shopping malls, and neighborhood streets [21] and that they prefer to engage in physical activities with their peers [22, 23], particularly with a partner [24]. Furthermore, previous research among older adults combining accelerometer data with GPS data, has shown that green environments resulted in greater moderate-to-vigorous PA [25] and a significant portion of daily PA is attributed to active commuting [26].
However, retrospective questionnaires are typically administered long after the activities occur, making it challenging for older adults to accurately recall and report about contexts and their affective and physical states during past activities, which may induce recall bias. Additionally, while GPS and accelerometer research can provide accurate data on location and PA, it does not capture affective and physical states during the activity. Furthermore, retrospective questionnaires that are administered only once cannot capture within-subject variations. Understanding these temporal fluctuations may provide valuable insights for developing health behavior change strategies, allowing for anticipation of real-time dynamics of determinants rather than viewing them as stable within individuals. EMA addresses this issue by collecting real-time data on contexts and affective states during activities. Previous EMA research using time-based prompts, where surveys are administered at fixed or random times throughout the day, has shown that constructs like fatigue are not stable and can fluctuate throughout the day [27]. However, in a time-based EMA study, individuals are prompted at fixed or randomly allocated times during the day [28]. This approach may result in missing many PA events, especially in populations where PA events are rare, such as in many older adult populations. Thus, the probability of prompting a questionnaire during a bout of PA is small leading to few opportunities to assess contexts and physical and affective states during older adults’ PA.
A promising method to examine within-subject variations and to perform simultaneous assessments of contexts, and physical and affective states during PA is sensor-triggered event-based EMA [28]. This method allows researchers to repeatedly capture real-time contexts and physical and affective states during or immediately after a specific event (e.g., short bout of PA), without inducing recall bias [29, 30].
Previous research using time-based EMA in adults (aged 25 years and older) already showed that affect during PA was related to the contexts in which the activity was performed [31]. Engaging in PA with other individuals resulted in higher levels of positive affect compared to solitary PA. For physical contexts, higher negative affect was reported when engaging in PA indoors as compared with outdoors [6, 31]. Thus, environments that evoke positive emotions may enhance PA promotion. In a study involving adults, the combination of walking-triggered electronic diaries and GPS data revealed significant variations in affective states influenced by social and physical environmental factors [32]. Despite these advancements, no research has described momentary affect in different physical and social contexts during older adults’ everyday lives.
The objectives of the current event-based EMA study were (1) to describe the contexts, affective (i.e., negative and positive affect) and physical states (i.e., pain and fatigue) of older adults during PA in a naturalistic setting as well as their variation within and between individuals, (2) to explore how these constructs vary over time (i.e., during PA episodes), and (3) to describe affect during PA according to the physical and social context. It is hypothesized that older adults primarily engage in PA in outdoor settings with peers. Contexts, affective and physical states are expected to show significant variability within PA episodes. Furthermore, positive affect during PA is anticipated to be higher in supportive social settings (e.g., engaging in activity with others) or pleasant physical contexts (outdoors in green spaces), whereas negative affect is expected to be lower in such environments.
Methods
Participants
Between March and October 2022, 92 healthy, community-dwelling older adults (65+), including those who reached the age of 65 during the year of the study, were recruited in Flanders, Belgium, through purposeful convenience sampling via organizations for older adults and social media. Purposeful sampling was employed to achieve a heterogeneous sample, considering socio-demographic factors such as age and gender. Participants were excluded from the study if they had been diagnosed with cognitive impairments (e.g., mild cognitive impairment, dementia) or were not able to walk at least 100 m independently. An equal distribution across gender and age groups was pursued.
Procedures
An intensive longitudinal study was conducted using sensor-triggered event-based EMA during seven consecutive days. Prior to the monitoring period, participants were visited at home for a full explanation of the study protocol and completion of a socio-demographic and health status questionnaire. Participants were instructed to download the custom-made mobile application ‘HealthReact’ onto their smartphone and to consistently wear a Fitbit activity tracker (Inspire 2 or Ionic) on the non-dominant wrist. Those without a smartphone were provided with a Motorola G30 or Motorola E20 phone and offered a brief training to use these devices. Participants were asked to charge the Fitbit activity tracker and smartphone at night.
During seven days, participants received sensor-triggered event-based EMA surveys after each short bout of PA, defined as at least five minutes of continuous walking or running, with a maximum of six questionnaires per day. These questionnaires were delivered by the HealthReact mobile application (version 1.62) [33]. This application is designed for conducting time- and event-based EMA studies and enables researchers to automatically trigger an EMA survey after a predefined event of PA, as measured by Fitbit. The specific triggering criterion for the EMA survey was set at a minimum of 60 steps per minute for five consecutive minutes, ensuring a high sensitivity (approximately 99%) and specificity (approximately 98%) in accurately identifying periods of sustained walking [34]. However, since the total number of triggered surveys per participant was lower than expected (i.e., on average seven events per participant during the entire monitoring period) for the first twelve participants, we adapted the protocol and decided to allow one outlier (i.e., one minute with fewer than 60 steps) during the five-minute walk for the remaining participants. To prevent disruption of the ongoing walking bout, the EMA surveys were only dispatched after two minutes of physical inactivity, which was defined as less than 10 steps per minute. However, since Fitbit syncs with the Fitbit app only every 15 min, there may have been a delay between the completion of the walk and the detection of the event. To accommodate for this delay, the maximum time interval to detect a potential walking event in the past was set at 20 min. Furthermore, to obtain a proportional distribution of prompts across the day and to avoid overburdening participants within a short time frame, the minimum time interval between two prompts was set at 60 min.
Completing the EMA survey required approximately two minutes. To allow sufficient time for participants to respond, the EMA survey remained accessible for 30 min. Participants were reminded to complete the questionnaire every 10 min, and during the last 5 min before the EMA survey expired, they received reminders every minute. At the conclusion of the seven-day measurement period, Fitbit activity trackers and any provided smartphones were collected again. The study was reported according to the STROBE checklist (see Supplementary file 1). In addition, relevant aspects from the CREMAS checklist, such as latency, were also considered to address the specifics of EMA methodology [35].
Description of materials
Baseline questionnaire
At baseline, each participant completed a paper-and-pencil questionnaire to assess socio-demographic and health-related variables, including age, gender, height, weight, educational level, marital status, number of (grand)children and pets and household situation (i.e., living alone or with others). Generic health status was assessed using the PROMIS-29 [36], which includes a Visual Analog Scale (VAS) score to assess pain. Considering the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic at the time of data collection, participants were also asked if the pandemic had affected their current PA levels and social interactions.
Sensor-triggered event-based EMA survey
In the sensor-triggered event-based EMA survey, participants’ physical activities, accompanying contexts, as well as their affective and physical states were evaluated. First, participants were asked to indicate whether or not they were active before the prompt was triggered. If the participants did not confirm being physically active before the trigger, the EMA survey was terminated. If the EMA survey continued, participants were asked about the type of activity they were performing, with answer categories including: (1) “leisure walking,” (2) “leisure biking,” (3) “walking for transport,” (4) “biking for transport,” (5) “household activities,” (6) “gardening,” (7) “shopping,” (8) “sport or exercise,” (9) “playing with grandchild,” or (10) “other.” If participants chose the answer category “other”, they were asked to specify what type of activity they were performing.
Next, physical and social contexts were assessed. Specifically, participants were asked whether they were indoors or outdoors during the PA event. Those who had been active indoors were asked to specify in which of the following indoor places they had been active: (1) “home,” (2) “shop,” (3) “home of relatives,” (4) “in a healthcare facility (e.g. with a GP or other),” (5) “in a public space (e.g., library, etc.),” or (6) “other.” For those who were active outdoors, answer categories included: (1) “in a natural environment,” (2) “in a built environment,” (3) “in a private environment (e.g., garden or terrace),” or (4) “other.” If participants responded “other”, they received the follow-up question, “Where specifically were you indoors/outdoors?”.
To gather information about the social context, participants were asked if they had engaged in PA alone or with others. For those who had been active with others, the following answer categories were provided: (1) “pets,” (2) “partner,” (3) “child(ren),” (4) “grandchild(ren),” (5) “other relatives,” (6) “friend(s),” (7) “treating physicians (e.g., GP, nurses, etc.),” (8) “neighbors,” (9) “acquaintances,” or (10) “other.” If participants chose the answer category “other”, specification was asked.
Next, participants’ affect during the PA event was examined using a dimensional approach, guided by Russell’s circumplex model of affect [11]. This framework conceptualizes affect along the dimension of valence, ranging from positive to negative. To capture these dimensions, we assessed enthusiasm and happiness as indicators of positive valence, and anxiety and nervousness as indicators of negative valence For instance, the participants were asked: “How enthusiastic were you while engaging in PA?”. Responses were rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from (0) “not at all” to (7) “very much.” These items were selected from the Experience Sampling Method (ESM) by Philippe Delespaul’s research team at the University of Maastricht [27, 37]. Finally, physical states experienced during the event were evaluated through assessments of pain and fatigue. For instance, the participants were asked “How much pain did you have while engaging in PA?”. Responses were recorded on a scale from (0) “not at all” to (7) “very much.”
All items were selected based on existing scientific literature [27, 38], and the results of a small-scaled elicitation study we conducted prior to the current event-based EMA study. Within this study, modal salient beliefs (i.e., the most commonly held beliefs about PA within the target group) were identified with six non-active older adults using qualitative interviews, and potential activities to be surveyed during the EMA study were explored using an activity diary [39, 40]. This way, all items were tailored as closely as possible to the target group.
Data processing and statistical analysis
EMA survey responses, time stamps and Fitbit-recorded steps in minute epochs were extracted from HealthReact. To identify potential technical issues, time stamps of Fitbit syncs were also extracted. Participants without EMA data were not included in the analyses. Descriptive statistics were computed to summarize sample characteristics and daily steps.
In addition, metrics were calculated, including the number of surveys sent, the number of completed surveys, the number of surveys in which the behavior was confirmed through the validation question, and the incidence of technical issues encountered by participants. In addition, the true positive rate (i.e., the proportion of surveys that were truly triggered by walking events divided by the total triggered surveys that were answered) was calculated. In addition, the prompt latency (i.e., the time between the last sync and when participants began responding) was calculated. However, the version of HealthReact used in this study did not log the exact timestamp of when a survey was prompted but only recorded the time when participants began their responses. We approximated the prompt timing by identifying the timestamp of the device sync that occurred after the stepping event but before participants started responding to the EMA survey.
To reach the first objective, descriptive statistics were calculated to quantify the physical and social contexts and affective (i.e., negative and positive affect) and physical states (i.e., pain and fatigue) of older adults during PA. The positive affect score was computed as the mean of ‘enthusiasm’ and ‘happiness,’ while the score for negative affect was calculated as the mean of ‘nervousness’ and ‘anxiety.’ In EMA questions where participants ticked the ‘other’ category, their specifications were recoded using one of the provided answer categories whenever possible. In addition, intercept-only generalized mixed models were fitted using the lme4 package [41] to investigate the within- and between-subject variance of the constructs. These models take into account the hierarchical data structure of the data (i.e., three levels: repeated measurements nested within days, within individuals). Since the physical and social contexts were defined as a binary outcome variable (i.e., outdoors vs. indoors and alone vs. not alone, respectively), logistic mixed models were conducted to examine variations in physical and social context. Generalized linear mixed models were performed to investigate the variation in physical states (Poisson Log Hurdle Model) and affective states (Gamma Log Hurdle Models for negative affect, and Gamma Log Models for positive affect). Variance and link functions were selected based on Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC). The selection of Hurdle Models was motivated by an overabundance of zero values in the outcome variables pain, fatigue, and negative affect. These variables were dichotomized as absent or present to run the first part of the Hurdle Models. To assess within-subject variability, participants that did not complete at least three EMA surveys (N = 8) were excluded from analysis. The between and within subjects variances of positive affect were estimated by calculating the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC). In addition, given the non-normal distribution of the other outcome variables, levels of between and within variance of contexts, physical complaints and negative affect were calculated using the simulation approach [42]. The within-subject variance includes both within-day and between-day variance, as the simulation approach does not distinguish between these two components.
To achieve the second objective and investigate how contexts and physical and affective states fluctuate during PA episodes, time was included in the intercept-only models as an independent variable with three categories: times between 6 am and 11:59 am were considered as ‘morning’, between 12 pm and 5:59 pm as ‘afternoon’, and between 6 pm and 12 am as ‘evening’ (= reference category). When comparing morning to afternoon, afternoon served as the reference category. We assessed the models’ assumptions through visual examination of residual-versus-fit plots and normal probability plots of standardized residuals. An example of a linear mixed model with positive affect as dependent variable and time as categorical independent variable: positive_affect_gamma_log = glmer(posaffectR ~ timeR + (1|ID/Day), family = Gamma(link="log”), data = EMA_validated).
Finally, to reach the third objective, two generalized linear mixed models were run to describe affect during PA according to the physical and social context. We utilized a Gamma Log Hurdle Model for negative affect and a Gamma Log Model for positive affect. An interaction effect for the physical and social context was incorporated into the model, since it was hypothesized that the effects might be interdependent. Analyses were performed using R (version 4.3.1). P-values below 0.05 were considered statistically significant.
Sample size
The sample size was determined to prevent overfitting rather than to achieve a specific power level [43]. In line with guidelines for regression models, a minimum of ten observations per predictor was considered sufficient [44]. For binary outcome models, based on observations from our study, where 19% of participants engaged in indoor PA and 37% engaged in PA with others, and accounting for an expected 80% compliance rate among older adults in EMA studies, we estimated that recruiting 66 participants would be adequate [45].
Results
Figure 1 provides a flowchart illustrating participant recruitment and exclusion. Initially, 92 individuals were recruited to participate in the study. However, four participants did not complete the one-week assessment period (e.g., because of experiencing too much stress due to study participation). Additionally, nine participants received no surveys at all, primarily due to synchronization problems between the Fitbit device and the Fitbit app. From the remaining 79 participants who received at least one survey, seven participants did not respond to any survey. This resulted in a final analytic sample of 72 participants, completing a total of 718 EMA surveys.
Table 1 presents the socio-demographic and health characteristics of the analytic sample. The median age was 71 (Q1 = 68, Q3 = 77) years and mean BMI was 26.3 ± 3.8 kg/m². Additionally, the sample included participants with a diverse range of educational backgrounds, relatively low pain scores, and high levels of PA (Table 1).
In total 884 EMA surveys were sent, 718 were responded (compliance rate 81.2%) and 666 were confirmed as being triggered during/just after PA. On average, two EMA surveys (median = 2; Q1 = 1, Q3 = 3) were triggered and answered per participant per day, with one survey (median = 1; Q1 = 1, Q3 = 3) confirmed daily. A detailed overview of the number of triggered surveys during the monitoring period is provided in Table 2. The true positive rate (i.e., proportion of surveys that were truly triggered by walking events divided by the total triggered surveys that were answered) was 91.32%. This means that 8.68% of the triggers were misclassified, with prompts being issued without being preceded by five minutes of walking. Additionally, participants self-reported being physically active in 92.76% of the EMA surveys, and the median latency to answer the EMA survey was 3.13 min (IQR = 15.34).
Objective 1: describing the contexts, affective and physical states of older adults during PA
The descriptive statistics and variability in contexts, affective and physical states during PA are displayed in Table 3. The three primary types of physical activities were: (1) walking for transport (27.7%), (2) leisure walking (19.5%), and (3) gardening (18.4%). Additionally, the majority of PA events occurred alone (62.7%) and outdoors (81.1%). When participants engaged in physical activities with someone else, it was primarily with their partner (54.6%). The participants’ own homes emerged as the predominant indoor location for physical activities (56.0%). Considering outdoor PA, the majority of participants engaged in PA within built environments (39.9%), with fewer indicating natural settings (31.8%) or private environments (26.5%). The median values for negative affect and physical states were low (i.e., 0), whereas the median for positive affect was relatively high (i.e., 6). Furthermore, the majority of the variance in physical and social context, affect, and fatigue was explained by within-subject differences, unlike the variance in pain.
Objective 2: describing the fluctuation of contexts, affective and physical states
Supplementary file 2 gives an overview of the results of the generalized mixed models we conducted to investigate how contexts, physical and affective states fluctuate during PA episodes. First, a generalized logistic mixed model was generated to investigate the odds of engaging in PA with someone else and outdoors. The odds of engaging in PA with someone else were 60% (95% CI: 26–78%, p < 0.01) lower in the morning compared to in the afternoon. No significant differences in the odds of engaging in PA with someone else were found when comparing activities between morning and evening or between afternoon and evening. The odds of engaging in PA outdoors are on average 2.9 times higher in the morning (95% CI: 109–713%, p = 0.03) and 2.8 times higher in the afternoon (95% CI: 175–683%, p = 0.03) compared to the evening. No significant differences in odds of engaging in PA outdoors were found between morning and afternoon activities.
Furthermore, Supplementary file 2 presents the results of a Hurdle Model to investigate the fluctuation of negative affect during PA episodes at various times of the day. Time of the day does not influence the likelihood of experiencing negative affect during an event. However, among events during which negative affect was experienced, the level of negative affect was highest in the evening. More specifically, among events during which negative affect was experienced, those occurring in the morning and afternoon were associated with an average reduction in negative affect of 28% (95% CI: 13–41%, p < 0.01) and 33% (95% CI: 18–45%, p < 0.01), respectively, compared to events that occurred in the evening. There was no significant effect between morning and afternoon levels. Furthermore, no significant time fluctuations were found for positive affect.
Regarding the physical complaints, the Hurdle Model did not reveal significant fluctuations in pain and fatigue over time. However, among events during which fatigue was experienced, a trend towards significance was observed. More specifically, events occurring in the morning were associated with an average 16% (95% CI: -2-30%, p = 0.07) lower level of fatigue compared to events that occurred in the afternoon and 25% (95% CI: -2-45%, p = 0.07) lower level of fatigue compared to events that happened in the evening. There was no significant difference in level of fatigue between afternoon and evening activities.
Objective 3: describing affect during PA according to the physical and social context
The third aim was to characterize the affective states during PA according to the physical and social context (see Table 4). Regarding the results for positive affect, the Gamma Log Model showed no significant interaction effect between the physical context and social context. Upon excluding the interaction effect, the Gamma Log Model showed no significant association between physical context (exp(b) = 1.27, 95% CI [0.93–1.13], p = 0.64) or social context (exp(b) = 1.06, 95% CI [0.98–1.14], p = 0.12) and positive affect.
For negative affect, the Logistic Hurdle Model showed no significant interaction effect between the physical and social context on the likelihood of experiencing negative affect. However, among those who experienced negative affect, a significant interaction effect was found in the Gamma Log Model between the physical and social context on the level of negative affect. This indicates that the relationship between negative affect and the social context significantly depends on the physical context. Activities performed alone are associated with lower levels of negative affect compared to when done with someone else, but only for outdoor activities. Conversely, this association is reversed for indoor activities; activities performed with others indoors are associated with lower levels of negative affect compared to activities performed alone.
Discussion
This study aimed to (1) describe the physical and social contexts, and the affective and physical states during PA among older adults, (2) evaluate how these constructs fluctuate during PA episodes, and (3) describe affective states during PA according to the context.
Main results
Physical and social contexts
This study found that nearly half of all detected PA events were performed in the form of walking, whether for leisure or commuting purposes. Besides walking, our results suggest that participants mainly perform activities that can be easily integrated into daily life, such as gardening and household chores. This observation aligns with previous research using traditional questionnaires [46, 47]. These activities are well-known for their health benefits in older adults and do not require planning or structuring as part of an exercise, as they occur naturally in daily life [47]. However, activities like these have been declining over the years due to technological innovations and digitalization, which reduce the need for physical effort in daily tasks (e.g., decreased active commuting, digital transformation of leisure activities.) [48]. Therefore, it is essential to continue promoting these daily activities as a simple but effective way to improve and maintain physical health and well-being, especially in an aging society.
In addition, our study revealed a predominant pattern of older adults engaging in physical activities alone. This pattern is inconsistent with existing literature indicating older adults’ preference for performing PA with others [23, 46]. Moreover, engaging in PA with others has been linked to improved mental well-being [49]. This may imply that while older adults may prefer to be physically active with others, they may not always have the opportunity to do so. However, our study also captured daily activities like gardening and household chores, which may be mainly performed alone and are often overlooked in current literature that predominantly concentrates on leisure PA [22, 23, 50]. Furthermore, traditional questionnaires might overlook these activities, as people typically associate PA with planned leisure activities. In addition, the sensor-triggered event-based EMA questionnaire was prompted already after a minimum of five minutes of sustained walking, making it plausible that shorter durations of PA are more likely to be pursued solitary. Such short bouts of PA may not have been captured in previous studies using traditional self-report questionnaires which often focus on activities lasting a minimum of ten minutes [51]. When activities were performed with someone else, it was usually with their partners which is in line with previous research [24, 52, 53].
Our results revealed that the majority of participants’ activities took place outdoors, with a significant portion occurring in built environments and natural environments. Indoor activities were predominantly conducted at home, underscoring the significance of both outdoor and home settings. This finding aligns with current literature, which highlights the importance of outdoor environments for PA and the role of the home environment in promoting regular activity, especially for older adults [54]. Interestingly, the study found that variability in physical and social contexts is largely due to within-subject differences. Differences in how, where, and with whom physical activities are performed vary significantly within the same individual across different times or contexts, rather than between different individuals.
Significant variations in these contexts throughout PA-episodes were observed, such as a decreased likelihood of engaging in PA with others during the morning compared to the afternoon. Morning routines are often centered around personal care, medical appointments, or solitary activities like walking or exercising alone to start the day [55]. By the afternoon, older adults might be more socially active, participating in group activities, community events, or meeting friends or family, leading to an increased likelihood of engaging in physical activities with others. In addition, the probability of participating in outdoor activities was lowest in the evening. This underrepresentation of evening outdoor activities may be due to higher perceived safety during daylight hours [56].
Affective and physical states
All participants consistently reported a relatively high level of positive affect and a low level of negative affect during PA events. Additionally, our study sample exhibited minimal physical complaints, such as pain and fatigue. This aligns with previous time-based EMA research in older adults, which found that high positive affect and low negative affect are associated with subsequent PA [57,58,59]. However, our findings primarily consist of descriptive results during bouts of PA; positive affect might have been high even before the activity began, or individuals experiencing negative affect may have chosen not to engage in PA at all. Therefore, it can be assumed that if individuals experience pain or negative affect, they might be unlikely to engage in PA. Furthermore, this sample might be considered a healthy sample of older adults, free from any physical complaints or other inconveniences, reducing the likelihood of individuals to report physical complaints.
We triggered EMA surveys after at least five minutes of walking to capture as many physical activity bouts as possible. However, laboratory research has shown that positive affect can emerge as early as three minutes into physical activity. The relationship between physical activity and affect is complex, influenced by factors such as intensity, duration, fitness levels, and psychological states [12, 60]. While positive affect may take time to emerge, moderate-intensity physical activity is generally associated with feelings of well-being, enjoyment, and energy both during and after activity [60,61,62]. Furthermore, laboratory research indicates that positive affect typically appears early—often within the first three minutes—and remains stable or increases as activity continues, unless prolonged fatigue sets in [63, 64]. In contrast, high-intensity physical activity is often linked to initial negative affective states, such as discomfort or exertion, with positive affect emerging after the activity is completed [15]. These timing of these affective responses also depend on baseline affective states, with individuals starting in a negative mood showing more pronounced improvement, and on fitness levels, as better-conditioned individuals tend to experience positive affect more quickly and at higher intensities [13, 65].
Interestingly, the current study revealed that the variability in positive as well as negative affect and fatigue during PA can be explained by within-subject differences. This means that fluctuations in an individual’s affect and levels of fatigue during PA are not solely due to differences between individuals, but are significantly influenced by personal variations in how each person experiences PA. However, these states were assessed exclusively during or immediately after PA bouts. Therefore, the observed within-subject variability in positive and negative affect might be lower than what might occur across an entire day. Incorporating mixed sampling schemes (such as combining event-based and time-based EMA) could enhance the interpretation by also assessing affect during non-PA moments. In addition, our findings indicate that the time of day is significantly associated with negative affect (but not with positive affect and fatigue). Specifically, negative affect was found to be highest during evening events. In addition, for events where fatigue was experienced, a trend towards significance indicated that morning events were associated with lower levels of fatigue compared to those in the afternoon and evening. However, the variation in pain could not be attributed to within-subject differences but rather to between-subject differences. This might be explained by the generally low levels of pain observed both during PA and in general. A more diverse and heterogeneous sample in terms of health status might be needed for exploring the variation of this construct.
Additionally, the context in which PA occurs was associated with negative affect, but not with positive affect. Specifically, outdoor activities performed alone were associated with lower levels of negative affect compared to activities done with others. This might be explained by the fact that solitude in outdoor settings may provide a sense of relief or relaxation [66], potentially reducing negative emotions more effectively than when engaging in these activities with others. Outdoor environments, which are often associated with exposure to natural elements and green spaces, are expected to have a positive impact on mood [31, 67,68,69]. Furthermore, a study combining walking-triggered electronic diaries and GPS data in adults, revealed significant associations between affective states (e.g., energetic arousal and calmness) and both social interactions and amount of greenness in the environment [32]. Participants reported lower energetic arousal when walking alone or in areas with less greenness, while walking with someone in greener settings was linked to higher levels of calmness [32]. These results align with our observation that levels of negative affect were lower during outdoor activities, suggesting a restorative potential of natural environments. Conversely, the pattern reverses for indoor activities. Here, engaging in activities with others indoors is associated with lower levels of negative affect compared to performing the same activities alone. This implies that the social context in indoors settings, may provide emotional support and reduce feelings of negative affect more effectively than in outdoor settings. Social interaction may provide emotional support and companionship, potentially enhancing mood and reducing feelings of loneliness [31, 70]. Finally, the relationship between positive affect and social context did not depend on the physical context in which activities occur. However, positive affect was generally high during PA, which may have limited potential for further enhancement through social interaction and an outdoor setting. Furthermore, positive affect encompasses a broad range of emotions [71], and the measurement instruments we employed to assess it might not have been sensitive enough to detect subtle differences.
Recommendations for PA interventions and future research
In addition to the World Health Organization’ (WHO) PA guidelines, which recommend 150 min of moderate-intensity activity per week, the WHO also advises older adults to engage in muscle-strengthening, balance, and coordination exercises at least twice a week to help prevent falls [4]. The F.I.T.T. (Frequency, Intensity, Time, and Type) principles provide a framework for understanding and optimizing PA [72]. Frequency refers to how often PA is performed, intensity indicates the level of effort, time denotes the duration of activity, and type specifies the kind of activity. This framework is particularly useful for tailoring PA recommendations to individual needs and preferences. The predominance of walking and gardening as the most common types of activities in this study, suggests there may be potential to increase other types of physical activities, such as muscle-strengthening, balance, and coordination exercises. This could provide a valuable focus area for future interventions [73]. However, it’s important to note that the surveys in this study were triggered by stepping events, which primarily captured data on activities involving walking. This may have limited our ability to assess activities that do not involve stepping, such as muscle-strengthening exercises. Additionally, the median number of surveys triggered per day was low (i.e., one per day), suggesting that the frequency of PA events among older adults could be further improved. Regarding intensity, this study applied a threshold of at least 60 steps per minute to detect PA events. However, this threshold does not meet the benchmark for moderate-intensity PA, which is approximately 110 steps per minute. As a result, many captured events likely included light-intensity PA.
Future research could incorporate geographically-explicit ecological momentary assessment (GEMA) to address some of these limitations [74]. GEMA combines EMA methodologies with geolocation data, enabling a more precise understanding of how physical and social environments influence PA [75]. For example, using geolocation triggers, surveys could be tailored to capture activities occurring in specific contexts, such as gyms, parks, or homes, where non-stepping activities like muscle-strengthening or balance exercises might be more prevalent. GEMA could also identify patterns in environmental contexts, and how these factors interact with individual behaviors and affective and physical states [74]. Additionally, proximity detection within GEMA frameworks could assess the presence and quality of social interactions during PA episodes [76]. This integration could offer a more holistic view of how social and environmental contexts collectively impact both physical and affective states, paving the way for more targeted and effective interventions.
In addition, the sampling scheme employed in this study presents a limitation in capturing causal effects between PA and contextual, affective, and physical states. However, our objectives were explicitly of a descriptive nature. Future studies could investigate these causal effects by employing causal inference methods (e.g., directed acyclic graphs) and mixed sampling schemes, such as combining event-based and time-based EMA. This approach would enable the assessment of affective states before, during, and after PA, as well as during non-PA episodes,, providing a more comprehensive understanding of the dynamic interplay between PA, affective experiences, and contextual factors. Furthermore, by assessing these states exclusively during or immediately after PA bouts, we only captured the within-subject variability in positive and negative affect during PA episodes. The overall within-subject variability across an individual’s entire daily life may be higher than what was observed here, but this lies beyond the scope of the current paper.
In addition, the fluctuations in contexts throughout PA episodes suggest that older adults make deliberate choices about which physical activities they perform at different times, which should be taken into account when developing interventions aiming to promote PA among older adults [77]. However, it remains unclear whether these choices truly reflect older adults’ preferences. For example, older adults might actually prefer more opportunities for being physically active with others in the morning. Therefore, before developing interventions, it is essential to conduct qualitative research to understand whether the observed daily patterns align with the true preferences of older adults. Tailoring interventions to align with older adults’ variability in preferences has the potential to enhance the success of future efforts in motivating them to become or remain physically active. Just-In-Time Adaptive Interventions (JITAIs) are such a potential intervention strategy that can leverage contextual information to send encouraging prompts at the right time and context [78]. Consequently, JITAIs could suggest activities in the afternoon that can be performed with others. In addition, since the likelihood of engaging in PA outdoors is higher in the morning and afternoon compared to in the evening, individuals could be encouraged to spend time outdoors during daytime. Furthermore, strategies could be explored to promote PA outdoors in the evening. Finally, the current study revealed that fatigue was lowest during PA events happening in the morning. Prompts or reminders for PA could be sent during the morning, when fatigue levels are lowest, to capitalize on the individual’s higher energy levels and likelihood of engagement. This could involve sending motivational messages or providing suggestions for morning exercise routines tailored to the individual’s preferences and capabilities.
Limitations and strengths
This study entails several limitations. First, our study sample was recruited through purposeful convenience sampling, which may have restricted the generalizability of our findings. Additionally, the sample included relatively active older adults with a slightly lower BMI (25.9) compared to the general population in Belgium (26.3), potentially resulting in lower levels of pain and fatigue [79]. Similarly, the average daily step count in our sample (8476 steps) was notably higher than what is typically observed among the general older adult population in Belgium, where only 20.5% (95% CI: 18.1–22.9) meet the PA guidelines. These differences suggest that our participants may represent a healthier and more active subset of the older adult population, potentially influencing the observed associations and limiting the applicability of our findings to less active or more diverse populations. Second, technical challenges, including synchronization issues between Fitbit and HealthReact, as well as a limited familiarity with smartphones among the participants, may have resulted in the omission of certain PA events. For example, seven participants who completed the seven-day monitoring period did not receive any sensor-triggered event-based EMA prompts due to technical issues. This may potentially have led to an underrepresentation of events and their associated contexts and affective and physical states in our study. In addition, participants who provided a higher number of responses may have a disproportionate influence on the results compared to those who completed only a few EMA surveys. To maximize inclusivity, smartphones were provided to participants who did not own one. However, a recently published paper including the data of the current study, showed that those without their own smartphones exhibited lower compliance rates [45]. Third, the definition of an event in terms of steps inevitably excludes other forms of PA, such as cycling, swimming and strength or flexibility exercises. Although Fitbit does not convert cycling to steps [80], our observations revealed instances where Fitbit registered steps during bike riding, potentially attributed to repetitive wrist movements or uneven terrains. This phenomenon contributes to the underrepresentation of cycling compared to walking as a reported activity. In addition, strength training typically involves stationary movements that do not generate the required number of steps to trigger the EMA survey. Despite thorough protocol testing, it is important to recognize that our technological choices may have resulted in missed events. These challenges highlight the need for clear guidelines regarding methodological aspects, including sampling type, prompt frequency, monitoring period, devices and EMA platforms, as well as technology choices such as start and end rules when implementing sensor-triggered event-based sampling. Fourth, participants were given a 30-minute window to allow sufficient time to complete the EMA questionnaire. However, it is important to acknowledge that this extended time frame may have introduced limited recall bias in participant responses. Fifth, it is important to acknowledge that some participants reported still being affected by COVID-19 during data collection. However, data collection spanned from March to October 2022 and all restrictions were dropped on March 7, 2022. Sixth, the primary objective was to describe contexts, affective and physical states during PA. It was not our purpose to examine causal effects of contexts on affect. Future research using intensive longitudinal data combined with causal inference methods (e.g., directed acyclic graphs) are needed to elucidate causal relationships between these constructs [81]. Finally, participants were not explicitly screened for mental health disorders beyond cognitive impairments. Since mental health conditions such as depression and anxiety can influence both PA patterns and affective states, future studies should consider including mental health assessments to better understand their potential moderating effects.
Despite its limitations, this study also encompasses several strengths. First, this study employs sensor-triggered event-based EMA, an innovative methodology in PA research. This approach enables researchers to repeatedly capture experiences, emotions and contexts during or immediately after a specific event (e.g., short bout of PA) in real-time. This design enabled us to capture short during physical activities, which are often overlooked while using traditional questionnaires [82, 83]. Second, this is one of the first non-self-initiated event-based EMA studies conducted among older adults. Despite potential concerns about the feasibility of conducting mobile-based EMA studies in older adults with no prior experience with smartphones [45], satisfactory response rates of 82.2% were observed in the current study. While technical issues led to some participant loss and missing data, emphasizing the importance of thorough smartphone usage instructions, our results align with prior studies demonstrating the viability of EMA in older populations [27, 84,85,86]. Thirdly, as data collection spanned from March to October 2022, our study captured information across diverse weather conditions. This extended time frame enhances the generalizability of our findings, albeit with the exception of the winter season.
Conclusion
In conclusion, activities that can be easily integrated into older adults’ daily life, such as walking for transport, gardening, and household chores, are most common. Furthermore, we found that they predominantly engage in physical activities alone, particularly in outdoor settings, which may contradict existing literature suggesting a preference for social interaction during PA. This discrepancy suggests a possible gap between opportunities and preferences, warranting further qualitative research to explore whether current activity patterns align with older adults’ true preferences. Moreover, a substantial portion of the variability in affect, fatigue, and the physical and social context is driven by within subject variance. The study also highlights the association of context and time of day on affective states during PA, with negative affect during PA peaking in the evening and fatigue being lowest in the morning. These insights could enhance the effectiveness of PA interventions by enabling the development of real-time, context-sensitive support strategies tailored to an individual’s current state and context. However, the descriptive nature of this study limits the ability to draw causal conclusions and offers no insight into the direction of the relationship between these constructs and PA. Future sensor-triggered event-based EMA studies should collect data both during active and inactive episodes, incorporating a within-person encouragement design [87]. This design involves randomly assigning EMA prompts within participants that include varying levels of encouragement (e.g., prompts encouraging walking, or sitting down). By applying this approach, future research can gain a more comprehensive understanding of the causal relationships between PA and contexts, affective and physical states.
Data availability
The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article is available in the Open Science Framework repository, [https://osf.io/94tcb/].
Abbreviations
- EMA:
-
Ecological momentary assessment
- BMI:
-
Body mass index
- SD:
-
Standard deviation
- JITAI:
-
Just-in-time-adaptive interventions
- OR:
-
Odds ratio
- CI:
-
Confidence interval
References
Lee IM, Shiroma EJ, Lobelo F, Puska P, Blair SN, Katzmarzyk PT. Effect of physical inactivity on major non-communicable diseases worldwide: an analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. Lancet. 2012;380(9838):219–29.
Jadhav RA, Maiya GA, Umakanth S, Shivashankara KN. Effect of a customized physical activity Promotion Program on Visceral Fat and Glycemic parameters in individuals with prediabetes: a Randomized Controlled Trial. J Phys Activity Health. 2024;21(2):164–70.
McTiernan A, Friedenreich CM, Katzmarzyk PT, Powell KE, Macko R, Buchner D, et al. Physical activity in Cancer Prevention and Survival: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019;51(6):1252–61.
World Health O. WHO guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2020 2020.
Keadle SK, McKinnon R, Graubard BI, Troiano RP. Prevalence and trends in physical activity among older adults in the United States: a comparison across three national surveys. Prev Med. 2016;89:37–43.
Papini NM, Yang C-H, Do B, Mason TB, Lopez NV. External contexts and movement behaviors in ecological momentary assessment studies: a systematic review and future directions. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2023;16(1):337–67.
Barnett DW, Barnett A, Nathan A, Van Cauwenberg J, Cerin E, on behalf of the Council on E. Built environmental correlates of older adults’ total physical activity and walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. 2017;14(1):103.
Lindsay Smith G, Banting L, Eime R, O’Sullivan G, van Uffelen JGZ. The association between social support and physical activity in older adults: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. 2017;14(1):56.
Higgins ET. Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Psychol Rev. 2006;113(3):439.
Staddon JE, Cerutti DT. Operant conditioning. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:115–44.
Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS. The circumplex model of affect: an integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17(3):715–34.
Magnan RE, Kwan BM, Bryan AD. Effects of current physical activity on affective response to exercise: physical and social-cognitive mechanisms. Psychol Health. 2013;28(4):418–33.
Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Ciccolo JT, Lewis BA, Albrecht AE, Marcus BH. Acute affective response to a moderate-intensity exercise stimulus predicts physical activity participation 6 and 12 months later. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2008;9(3):231–45.
Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Jennings EG, Marcus BH. Does affective Valence during and immediately following a 10-Min Walk Predict Concurrent and Future Physical Activity? Ann Behav Med. 2012;44(1):43–51.
Ekkekakis P, Parfitt G, Petruzzello SJ. The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they Exercise at different intensities. Sports Med. 2011;41(8):641–71.
Brand R, Ekkekakis P. Affective–reflective theory of physical inactivity and exercise. German J Exerc Sport Res. 2018;48(1):48–58.
Timm I, Giurgiu M, Ebner-Priemer U, Reichert M. The within-Subject Association of Physical Behavior and Affective Well-being in Everyday Life: a systematic literature review. Sports Med. 2024;54(6):1667–705.
Liao Y, Shonkoff ET, Dunton GF. The Acute relationships between Affect, physical feeling States, and physical activity in Daily Life: a review of current evidence. Front Psychol. 2015;6.
Meredith SJ, Cox NJ, Ibrahim K, Higson J, McNiff J, Mitchell S et al. Factors that influence older adults’ participation in physical activity: a systematic review of qualitative studies. Age Ageing. 2023;52(8).
Shiffman S, Hufford M, Hickcox M, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD. Remember that? A comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking lapses. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997;65(2):292–300.
Herbolsheimer F, Mahmood A, Michael YL, Chaudhury H. Everyday walking among older adults and the Neighborhood built environment: a comparison between two cities in North America. Front Public Health. 2020;8:564533.
Burton NW, Khan A, Brown WJ. How, where and with whom? Physical activity context preferences of three adult groups at risk of inactivity. Br J Sports Med. 2012;46(16):1125.
Chong TW, Doyle CJ, Cyarto EV, Cox KL, Ellis KA, Ames D, et al. Physical activity program preferences and perspectives of older adults with and without cognitive impairment. Asia Pac Psychiatry. 2014;6(2):179–90.
Smith GSE, Moyle W, Burton NW. Frequency of physical activity done with a companion: changes over seven years in adults aged 60 + living in an Australian Capital City. J Aging Health. 2023;35(9):736–48.
Pasanen S, Halonen JI, Gonzales-Inca C, Pentti J, Vahtera J, Kestens Y, et al. Changes in physical activity by context and residential greenness among recent retirees: longitudinal GPS and accelerometer study. Health Place. 2022;73:102732.
Pasanen S, Halonen JI, Pulakka A, Kestens Y, Thierry B, Brondeel R, et al. Contexts of sedentary time and physical activity among ageing workers and recent retirees: cross-sectional GPS and accelerometer study. BMJ Open. 2021;11(5):e042600.
Maes I, Mertens L, Poppe L, Crombez G, Vetrovsky T, Van Dyck D. The variability of emotions, physical complaints, intention, and self-efficacy: an ecological momentary assessment study in older adults. PeerJ. 2022;10:e13234.
Stone AA, Shiffman S. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavorial medicine. Ann Behav Med. 1994;16(3):199–202.
Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4:1–32.
Dunton GF. Ecological Momentary Assessment in Physical Activity Research. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2017;45(1):48–54.
Dunton GF, Liao Y, Intille S, Huh J, Leventhal A. Momentary assessment of contextual influences on affective response during physical activity. Health Psychol. 2015;34(12):1145–53.
Bollenbach L, Schmitz J, Niermann C, Kanning M. How do people feel while walking in the city? Using walking-triggered e-diaries to investigate the association of social interaction and environmental greenness during everyday life walking. Front Psychol. 2022;13.
Vetrovsky T, Kral N, Pfeiferova M, Kuhnova J, Novak J, Wahlich C, et al. mHealth intervention delivered in general practice to increase physical activity and reduce sedentary behaviour of patients with prediabetes and type 2 diabetes (ENERGISED): rationale and study protocol for a pragmatic randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2023;23(1):613.
Delobelle J, Lebuf E, Dyck DV, Compernolle S, Janek M, Backere F, et al. Fitbit’s accuracy to measure short bouts of stepping and sedentary behaviour: validation, sensitivity and specificity study. Digit Health. 2024;10:20552076241262710.
Liao Y, Skelton K, Dunton G, Bruening M. A systematic review of methods and procedures used in ecological momentary assessments of Diet and Physical Activity Research in Youth: an adapted STROBE checklist for reporting EMA studies (CREMAS). J Med Internet Res. 2016;18(6):e151.
Hays RD, Spritzer KL, Schalet BD, Cella D. PROMIS(®)-29 v2.0 profile physical and mental health summary scores. Qual Life Res. 2018;27(7):1885–91.
PAEG. D. Assessing schizophrenia in daily life: the experience sampling method. Maastricht: Datawyse / Universitaire Pers; 1995.
Koeneman MA, Verheijden MW, Chinapaw MJ, Hopman-Rock M. Determinants of physical activity and exercise in healthy older adults: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. 2011;8:1–15.
Shutton S, French DP, Hennings SJ, Mitchell J, Wareham NJ, Griffin S et al. Eliciting salient beliefs in Research on the theory of Planned Behaviour: the Effect of question wording. Current psychology: a Journal for diverse perspectives on diverse psychological issues. 2003;22(3):234–51.
Wolcott MD, Lobczowski NG. Using cognitive interviews and think-aloud protocols to understand thought processes. Curr Pharm Teach Learn. 2021;13(2):181–8.
Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1):1–48.
Devine S, Uanhoro JO, Otto AR, Flake JK. Approaches for quantifying the ICC in multilevel logistic models: a didactic demonstration. Collabra: Psychol. 2024;10(1).
Babyak MA. What you see may not be what you get: a brief, nontechnical introduction to overfitting in regression-type models. Psychosom Med. 2004;66(3):411–21.
Peduzzi P, Concato J, Kemper E, Holford TR, Feinstein AR. A simulation study of the number of events per variable in logistic regression analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49(12):1373–9.
Compernolle S, Vetrovsky T, Maes I, Delobelle J, Lebuf E, De Vylder F, et al. Older adults’ compliance with mobile ecological momentary assessments in behavioral nutrition and physical activity research: pooled results of four intensive longitudinal studies and recommendations for future research. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2024;21(1):92.
Samra PK, Rebar AL, Parkinson L, van Uffelen JGZ, Schoeppe S, Power D, et al. Physical activity attitudes, preferences, and experiences of regionally-based Australia adults aged 65 years and older. J Aging Phys Act. 2019;27(4):446–51.
Sanchez-Lopez J, Silva-Pereyra J, Fernández T, Alatorre-Cruz GC, Castro-Chavira SA, González-López M, et al. High levels of incidental physical activity are positively associated with cognition and EEG activity in aging. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(1):e0191561.
Moreno-Llamas A, García-Mayor J, De la Cruz-Sánchez E. The impact of digital technology development on sitting time across Europe. Technol Soc. 2020;63:101406.
Lam J, García-Román JS, Day. Solitary activities, and associations with Well-being among older adults. Journals Gerontology: Ser B. 2019;75(7):1585–96.
Gothe NP, Kendall BJ. Barriers, motivations, and preferences for physical activity among female African American older adults. Gerontol Geriatric Med. 2016;2:2333721416677399.
Booth M. Assessment of physical activity: an international perspective. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2000;71(Suppl 2):114–20.
Satariano WA, Haight TJ, Tager IB. Living arrangements and participation in leisure-time physical activities in an older population. J Aging Health. 2002;14(4):427–51.
Pettee KK, Brach JS, Kriska AM, Boudreau R, Richardson CR, Colbert LH, et al. Influence of marital status on physical activity levels among older adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38(3):541–6.
Meghani NAA, Hudson J, Stratton G, Mullins J. Older adults’ perspectives on physical activity and sedentary behaviour within their home using socio-ecological model. PLoS ONE. 2023;18(11):e0294715.
Copeland JL, Esliger DW. Accelerometer Assessment of physical activity in active, healthy older adults. J Aging Phys Act. 2009;17(1):17–30.
Knoop M, Stefani O, Bueno B, Matusiak B, Hobday R, Wirz-Justice A, et al. Daylight: What Makes Difference? Lighting Res Technol. 2020;52(3):423–42.
Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(10):496–502.
Maes I, Mertens L, Poppe L, Vetrovsky T, Crombez G, De Backere F, et al. Within-person associations of Accelerometer-assessed physical activity with time-varying determinants in older adults: time-based ecological momentary Assessment Study. JMIR Aging. 2023;6:e44425.
Dunton GF, Atienza AA, Castro CM, King AC. Using ecological momentary assessment to examine antecedents and correlates of physical activity bouts in adults age 50 + years: a pilot study. Ann Behav Med. 2009;38(3):249–55.
LePage ML, Crowther JH. The effects of exercise on body satisfaction and affect. Body Image. 2010;7(2):124–30.
Gauvin L, Rejeski WJ, Norris JL. A naturalistic study of the impact of acute physical activity on feeling states and affect in women. Health Psychol. 1996;15(5):391–7.
Hogan CL, Mata J, Carstensen LL. Exercise holds immediate benefits for affect and cognition in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(2):587–94.
Zenko Z, Ekkekakis P, Ariely D. Can you have your vigorous Exercise and enjoy it too? Ramping Intensity Down increases Postexercise, remembered, and forecasted pleasure. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;38(2):149–59.
Ekkekakis P, Petruzzello SJ. Acute aerobic exercise and affect: current status, problems and prospects regarding dose-response. Sports Med. 1999;28(5):337–74.
Baldwin AS, Kangas JL, Denman DC, Smits JA, Yamada T, Otto MW. Cardiorespiratory fitness moderates the effect of an affect-guided physical activity prescription: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Cogn Behav Ther. 2016;45(6):445–57.
Hansmann R, Hug S-M, Seeland K. Restoration and stress relief through physical activities in forests and parks. Urban Forestry Urban Green. 2007;6(4):213–25.
Li D, Zhai Y, Xiao Y, Newman G, Wang D. Subtypes of park use and self-reported psychological benefits among older adults: a multilevel latent class analysis approach. Landsc Urban Plann. 2019;190:103605.
Thompson Coon J, Boddy K, Stein K, Whear R, Barton J, Depledge MH. Does participating in physical activity in Outdoor Natural environments have a Greater Effect on Physical and Mental Wellbeing than Physical Activity Indoors? A systematic review. Environ Sci Technol. 2011;45(5):1761–72.
Han K-T. The effect of nature and physical activity on emotions and attention while engaging in green exercise. Urban Forestry Urban Green. 2017;24:5–13.
Berry DS, Hansen JS. Positive affect, negative affect, and social interaction. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1996;71(4):796–809.
Shiota MN, Sauter DA, Desmet PMA. What are ‘positive’ affect and emotion? Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2021;39:142–6.
Barisic A, Leatherdale ST, Kreiger N. Importance of frequency, intensity, Time and Type (FITT) in Physical Activity Assessment for Epidemiological Research. Can J Public Health. 2011;102(3):174–5.
Bauman A, Merom D, Bull FC, Buchner DM, Fiatarone Singh MA. Updating the evidence for physical activity: Summative Reviews of the Epidemiological evidence, prevalence, and interventions to promote active aging. Gerontologist. 2016;56(Suppl2):S268–80.
Chaix B. How daily environments and situations shape behaviors and health: momentary studies of mobile sensing and smartphone survey data. Health Place. 2020;61:102241.
Kirchner TR, Shiffman S. Spatio-temporal determinants of mental health and well-being: advances in geographically-explicit ecological momentary assessment (GEMA). Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(9):1211–23.
Fernandes A, Van Lenthe FJ, Vallée J, Sueur C, Chaix B. Linking physical and social environments with mental health in old age: a multisensor approach for continuous real-life ecological and emotional assessment. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2021;75(5):477.
Dunton GF, Atienza AA. The need for time-intensive information in Healthful Eating and Physical Activity Research: a timely topic. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109(1):30–5.
Nahum-Shani I, Smith SN, Spring BJ, Collins LM, Witkiewitz K, Tewari A, et al. Just-in-Time adaptive interventions (JITAIs) in Mobile Health: Key Components and Design principles for Ongoing Health Behavior support. Ann Behav Med. 2018;52(6):446–62.
Sciensano. health-interview-survey 2018 [Available from: https://www.sciensano.be/en/projects/health-interview-survey/hisia#lifestyle
Fitbit. 2024 [Available from: https://community.fitbit.com/t5/Community/ct-p/EN
Rohrer JM, Murayama K. These are not the effects you are looking for: causality and the Within-/Between-Persons distinction in Longitudinal Data Analysis. Adv Methods Practices Psychol Sci. 2023;6(1):25152459221140842.
Ryan DJ, Wullems JA, Stebbings GK, Morse CI, Stewart CE, Onambele-Pearson GL. Reliability and validity of the international physical activity questionnaire compared to calibrated accelerometer cut-off points in the quantification of sedentary behaviour and physical activity in older adults. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(4):e0195712.
Logan SL, Gottlieb BH, Maitland SB, Meegan D, Spriet LL. The physical activity scale for the Elderly (PASE) questionnaire; does it predict physical health? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2013;10(9):3967–86.
King AC, Hekler EB, Grieco LA, Winter SJ, Sheats JL, Buman MP, et al. Harnessing different motivational frames via mobile phones to promote daily physical activity and reduce sedentary behavior in aging adults. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(4):e62613.
Thai S, Page-Gould E, ExperienceSampler. An open-source scaffold for building smartphone apps for experience sampling. Psychol Methods. 2018;23(4):729–39.
Ramsey AT, Wetherell JL, Depp C, Dixon D, Lenze E. Feasibility and acceptability of Smartphone Assessment in older adults with cognitive and emotional difficulties. J Technol Hum Serv. 2016;34(2):209–23.
Schmiedek F, Neubauer AB. Experiments in the Wild: introducing the within-person encouragement design. Multivar Behav Res. 2020;55(2):256–76.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge all researchers and students involved in the data collection.
Funding
The current study has received financial support from the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) with project number 3G005520. JD was supported by a FWO PhD Fellowship (11M3623N). The funding bodies had no input in study design and the collection, analysis and interpretation of data and the writing of the article and the decision to submit it for publication.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
JD, DVD, SC and JVC designed the research question. JD collected and cleaned the data, performed the analysis, and wrote the first draft. JD, TV, SC, JVC, and DVD read, provided feedback and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All participants provided written informed consent before the start of the study. Ethical approval was obtained from the Ethical Committee of Ghent University Hospital (BC-09944).
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
Authors SC and DVD are members of the Editorial Board of International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. They were not involved in the journal’s peer review process of, or decisions related to, this manuscript.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Delobelle, J., Compernolle, S., Vetrovsky, T. et al. Contexts, affective and physical states and their variations during physical activity in older adults: an intensive longitudinal study with sensor-triggered event-based ecological momentary assessments. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 22, 30 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12966-025-01724-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12966-025-01724-9